DIK, SIMON C., Tite Theory of Funutional Grammar Part 2. Complex ant) Derived Constructions. Ed. Kees Hengeveld. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 1997. xx + 477 pp. ISBN: 3-11-015405-6. This secund pan of Tite Theory of Functional Gramrnar (TFG2) sees the light more tan two years after te death uf its author, Simon C. Dik, at te age uf 55. The publication uf Dik’s pusthumous grammar is made possible wit te assistance of Kces Hcngeveld, Dik’s disciple and successur at the Vrije Universiteit van Amsterdam. The text as it is published intends tu reflect Simon Dik’s views as closely as possible; with ube exception of chapters 16 tu 18, ube draft versions of alí the chapters in uhis volume were discussed with te autor. It is a sequel tu pan 1 of Tite Theory of Functional Grammar (TEGI), published by Dik in 1989 and revised in 1997, tu which it makes constant reference and whose content is presuppused. ‘[bis second pan completes Simon Dik’s view of a model of description uf a natural language, by dcaling witb tbe funetional grammar of complex and derived cunstructions. It is designed tu be used “for advanced study of FG by thuse wbo alrcady have a reasonable knowledge of the basic framework of the theory” (TFGI: Preface). FG is a grammar wbich is basically centred un the sentence or, in Dik’s terms, predication. ‘[he predicatiun, buwever, is not viewed in isolation but in context. In general terms, FG atuempus tu describe a natural language as an instrument whicb can only be understood correctly as functioning in a wider, pragmatic setting. ‘[bis is reflected in the consideration of praginatics as “te all-encompassing framework within whicb semantics and syntax must be studicd” (TFGI:7). In panicular, both TFGJ and TFG2 aim tu describe and explain te linguistic expressions uf natural languages of any type in a way that is typologically, pragmatically andpsycholugically adequate. Ibis approacb may be compared wit oter functiunal-typological grammars such as those of Givón (cf. Givón 1984 and 1990) in that it attempts tu arrive at a universally valid characterisation of linguistic phenumena, wbilc recognising thc effect of extemal facturs which determine the nature of tbe commun properties of languages, and resurting tu genetic factors only wben alí other attempts at explaining the linguistic facts have failed. As a result uf this typulogical interest, ube grammars are enriched with examples from a wide scope of languages, ranging from Western (mainly English and Dutcb) tu other less common Australian ur African languages. In spite of being a functionally-oriented grammar, FO exhibits a higher degree uf formalisation than most utber functional approaches tu language. ‘[his is in part due tu the use of logic (for which Dik uses tbe term “Functional Logic”) which enables the derivation of new pieces of knowledgc from tbc knuwledge te mudel already possesses. As a result uf this, FO analyses of sentence structure often resemble dic notatiunal conventions used in formally-based analyses and, in particular. those of Chomskyan ‘[ransformatiunal-Generative grammar. Ibere exist points of convergende in the treatment of language structurc in botb grammatical framcworks, and transformational-generative cxplanations of specific linguistic Estudios Ingleses de la Universidad Complutense, 6,241-249. Madrid, 1998. 242 Reseñas phenomena are oftcn introduced tu illustrate tbe line of argumentation, althougb they are ultimately rejected in favour uf othcr arguments more compatible with the spirit of FO. Nonctbeless, Dik’s standpoint is absolutcly functional and, as Siewierska (1991:1) states, he “places FO firmly witbin the functional paradigm of linguistic theury which he repcatedly champions and unequivocally opposes tu the formal paradigm as represented by mainstream American linguists”. Almost equal in importance tu tbe grammars tbemselves (mainly, Dik 1978, 1989 and the present text of 1997 ‘) are the various written cuntributions of otber FO linguists 2, which provide applications of FO ideas tu different languages ur more detailed accounts of spccific topics. Many of these volumes carne tu ligbt as compilations uf selected papers of the FO conferences which have been being held in Europe every two years since 1986 (Antwerpen). ‘[hese volurnes hclp those interested in FO tu keep track of te devclopment of ideas and the more recent interests wiibin this theoretical model (cf. cg. Bolkestein et al. 1981, Huekstra et al., eds. 1981, Bolkestein et al., eds. 1985a and 1985b, van der Auwera & Goosscns, eds. 1987, Nuyts & de Schuttcr, eds. 1987, Hannay & Vester, eds. 1990, Connolly et al., eds. 1996). ‘[he prcsent book consists of eightcen chapters, dealing with thirtcen main topics involving complex asid derived constructions. Wbile TFGI mainly concentrated on te structure of main clauses, TFG2 discusses tbc most important linguistic phenomena uutside the boundaries of the clause proper. TFG2 can be further síructured in blocks by grouping together those chapters whicb concentrate un related issues, as will be seen in the more detailed discussiun of the contents of the volume wbicb follows. It would have been useful tu bave an introductory section at the beginning of the book with an uverview of te different phenomcna discussed in the text and an explanation of how tbe author envisaged the connection between TFGI and thc present volume. Instead, tbe autor starts witb the discussion in tbe first chapter of une uf thesc pbenurnena, predicate formation. In this chapter, Dik gives a survey of tbe most impurtant typcs of predicate formation míes as found across languages, by means uf wbich new predicates can be derived frum given predicates in a particular language. In FO each clause is described in terms of an abstract underlying clause structure (apredicateframe) wbich is mapped un tu the actual linguistic exprcssion by a system of expression míes. ‘[he constructiun of such a predicate brame first of alí requires a predicare, wbicb designates properties ur relations and wbicb is tu be applied tu an apprupriate number of terms, which designate entities (cf. tbe Preview un FO in 01k 19S9:4Sffl. An exasnplc of such a predicate formation rule is tbc Agent Nuun formatiun in English by means uf wbich from any action verb such as work we can derive tbe corresponding Agent Noun worker. In tbis chapter, Dik presents a modified approach tu predicate formation in which be not only describes the processes involved, but also gives an explanatory treatment of how tbcse processes can be functiunally understood. Cbapters 2 tu 7 and 9 may be grouped togetbcr since they are cuncemed with ube descriptiun in FO terms of linguistic operations such as subordinatiun, embedding and cuordination which extend beyund tbe simple clause in variuus ways. Cbapters 2, 3 and 4 focus un verbal restrictors. Restriclors were introduced in TFGJ tu designae elements which restrict or narruw duwn the set uf putential Reseñas 243 referents of tbe term within whicb tbey are embedded (TFGI:ll6ff, TFG2:27). Of alí te types of restrictors tbat may qualify a term, Dik concentrates un restrictors whicb are construed around verbal predicates or verbal restrictors (VRs). A typical example of a VR is a relative clause modifying a noun (tite man who is working in the garden). Tbe term verbal restrictor is rightly used by Dik in a wider sense than tbe subclass relative clause tu include rcstrictors whose verb may not be explicit (The school in tbe centre of town is not very good) but which can indeed be regarded as altemative formal expressions of a similar semantic configuration (Tite school which is in ube centre of town is nót very good). Wbereas chapter 2 discusses sume fundamental properties of VRs, chapter 3 gives a survey uf te typulogical variation across languages asid uf tbe ways tbese various types can be interpreted in terms of FO. After this detailed account of VRs, chapter 4 cuncentrates un the explanatury level, by raising a number of questions pertaining tu ube ways in wbicb the variety and tbe recurrent properties of VRs could be explained. Tbis procedure of addrcssing a number of questions, by way of hypoubeses, asid then discussing sume pussible answers tu tbcm from a functiunal graminar perspective is fulluwed by Dik throughout the whole book. ‘[bis is a useful methodological tuol whicb simplifies the task uf the reader in fullowing ube line of argumentation asid enduws tbc discussions witb tbeoretical rigour. Chaptcrs 5, 6 and 7 prcsent te FO vicw of embedded cunstructiosis. In chapter 5, Dik develops a semantic typology uf cmbedded constructions taking into accuunt te differcnt types of matrix predicate (or superordinate clause, cf. Downing & Locke 1992) and tbe semantic constraints whicb tbese matrix predicates may impuse un tbeir embedded complcments. ‘[be different types of cmbedded constructions outlined by Dik according tu tbe type of cumplex term tbey refer tu are: embedded predications, wbicb make reference tu a state of affair (John witnessed tbe changing of the guards), embedded propositions in wbicb reference is made tu a possible fact (JoItn knew tbat Mary bad failed tu sbow up) and embedded clauses, referring tu a speech act (John considered wby Peter bad failed tu sbow up) t ‘[he tcrm embedded construction is also used by Dik tu refer tu adverbial clauses which function as satellites (John will only take tite job ¡f nobody else is interested). Other functionally-oriented grammars, while not rcjecting ube constituency analysis for these adverbial clauses, present an alternative analysis which views tbese adverbial clauses as dependent, ratber than embedded, and which is considered preferable fur discourse (cf. Downing & Locke 1992). In this analysis, adverbial subordinate clauses aredistinguished from nominal asid adjectival clauses whicb are indeed embcdded asid function as arguments of the superordinate clause. The treatmcnt of embedded constructions cuntinues in chapter 6 witb a discussiun of funetiunal asid formal properties of these constructiosis. Witbin tbe functional parameters, Dik concentrates un different types uf functions wbicb the embedded construction may have: semantic functions (such as Agent, Goal or Zero), perspectiva! functions (ur syntactic functiuns, Subj and Obj) asid pragmatie functions (mainly Topic asid Focus). In FO, te predication in general receives te assignation of tbree levels of functions: semantie, syntactic (or perspectival) and pragmatic functions. ‘[bis differentiation of the threc levels of linguistic analysis is present in most functionally-orientcd grarnmars, starting witb Dane~, une of the first linguists whu postulates this three-level approacb tu language (cf. Dane~ 1966). In 244 Reseñas the discussion uf tbe formal properties uf embedded constructions, ube position of te construction is une of tbe features discussed wbich, as Dik describes, is affected by principies such as that of categorial complexity (or LIPOC, cf. TFG2: 127, TFGI:cb.16) and tbe iconicity principIe. Cbapter 7 completes tbe treatment of embedded cunstructions by cuncentrating un the different types uf these constructiosis wbicb can be fuund witbin and across languages. Dik provides a taxonomy wbich is intended tu have cross-linguistic validity and in whicb tbe first parameter used tu classify tbcse constructions is the presence of a finite or non-finite verb. Cbapter 9 concentrates un cuordination, by mainly looking at tbe different types of cuordinative relatiunships distinguished and how ube>’ can be treated in FG. In Dik’s cbaracterisatiun of cuordination importance is given tu tbe fact that the members uf the cuordination shuuld befunctionally equivalenr In many approaches tu tbis phenomenon the requiremcnt bas been tbat the members should be categorially equivalent (i.e. should be constituents belonging tu the same category), but Dik proves that, even ib ibis criterion is met, the result may still be an ungrammatical sentence containing cuordinated terms. Bis explanation is tbat cuordinated members sbould have ube same functions, not unly syntactic, but aNo semantie asid even pragmatic (fur a more detailed accuunt, see TFG2:192ff). Dik also rejects tbe traditional treatmcnt of cuordinate constructions as reductions of cuordinated fulí clause structures (cf cg. Quirk et al. 1972:ch. 9). Instead, be proposes a “Direct Approach” tu cuordination, more compatible with ube spirit of FO, which postulates “a cuordination of sub-clausal cunstituents in terms uf rules which directly multiply such constituents locally, in the position in which they occur in tbe clause structure” (TFG2:195). Otber issues conceming non-basic clausal structures sucb as negative polarity and interrogative clauses are described in cbapters 8 and 12, respectively. A related chapter is chapter 11 witb a detailed account of the illocutionary layer. In chapter 8 Dik considers negation (whicb be views as an operator4 ratber tban a satellite) frum various angles including what precisely is denied or negated, the pragmatic and semantic values of negation and the different strategies wbich may manifest it in formal exprcssion. Folluwing Lyons (1977), thc autbor makes a distiuctiun between predicational negation asid propositional negation un semantic and pragmatic grounds. ‘[he former reflects an objcctive statement of the nun-occurrence of sorne state of affair (John is not rich); tbe latter indicates a subjective denial of sume pre- established propusition (John is NOT rich —as the addressee secms tu imply). The first form of negatiun involves a case of New or Cumpletive Focus, the second a case uf Counter-Presuppositiunal Focus (cf. TEGJ: 13.4). Chapter 12 describes how different types of interrogative clauses can be treated ¡si FO, including tbe various responses which ma>’ be given tu questions. A large purtion of tbe chapter concentrates un Q-word questiuns wbich, for Dik, display te most interesting properties, especially in their interrelations with verbal restricturs (cf cbapters 2-4), un tbe une hasid, asid Cleft constructions (cf. cbapters 13-14), un tbe utber. Witb respect tu this second type of relationsbip, Dik’s statement tbat Q- wurd questiuns are a particular type uf Focus construction is bigbly questiunable. Focus constructiusis are defined in TFGJ:278 as “constructiosis whicb intrmnsically define a specific constituent as baving ube Fucus function”; tbe typical constructiun Reseñas 245 included in tbis gruup is tbe Clcft construction. It is true that there exist Q-word questions whicb take te form uf a Cleft constructiun (What was it that Peterfound in tIte garden?) but tus cannut be cunsidered te basic pattern, at least in ube mure common Wcstern lasiguages. Also worrying is tbe constant use of unnatural examples for whicb a clear context of uccurrence is difficult tu find ~. Mucb more successful is tbe discussion of the variuus types of responses and tbe distisiction between answers asid responses. Questions are sentence types witb interrogative illucutionary furce as tbeir basic illocution, but other derived illucutiosis (such as Excíamatiun, Request or Rheturical Questiun) can be also found in interrogatives by cunversiun of the interrogative illocutionary operator. Chapter 11 centres around diese basic asid derived illocutiusis of tbe different sentence types. The cbapter outlines tbe wurk of scbolars such as Austin and Searle in what is a useful review uf Speecb Act ‘[beory. Chapters 13 and 14 can be grouped togeuber in terms of their similar concem, the use of Focus constructiosis, special complex constructions wbich specifically have tbe function of bringing sume Focus constituent into prominence. Focus constructions are typically represented by Cleft constructions. ‘[hese include not unly “protutypical” Cleft and Pseudocleft sentences, but also identifying constructions with “classificatory” head nouns sucb as person, tIting, etc. in une of tbe arguments of tbc construction (TIte tbing Ifound is John’s watch). ‘[bis is In agreement with te treatment given tu both identifying and Cleft constructions and tbcir inclusion in the same gruup as Focus constructions in other related wurks (cf Moreno Cabrera 1987, Martínez Caro 1995:ch.5). Similarí>’, Dik rigbtly distinguishes between protutypical Clefts sucb as (1) and cunstructions sucb as (2): (1) a. It was JOHN with whom 1 went to New York. b. It was JOHN that 1 went tu New York witIt. (2) It was WITH JOHN that ¡ went to New York. by positing a different analysis for eacb of them. ‘[be embedded clause in (2) is not a relative constructiun as in tbe prototypical clefts, but a general subordisiate that- clause. In semantic tcrms, constructions of type (1) and (2) have also diffcrent typcs of prcdications: whereas (1) contains identifying constructiuns, (2) is a property- assigning une. In chapter 14, Dik discusses ubree mure specific pbenumena cunccming Focus constructiuns. 1 would likc tu draw te reader’s attention tu une of these issues, tbe proccss of “demarking” of Focus constructions, wbich can serve tu illustrate the use uf diachronic features in Dik’s fusictional explanations. By tbe “demarking” of Focus constructions, the author understands certain grammatical phenomena in (mutually unrelated) languages through whicb “an originally marked Focus cunstruction is increasingly used in cunditions in which no special focusing is called for, and finally ends up as dxc pragmatically neutral, unmarked clause type uf tbe language invulved” (TFG2:325). mis isa process typically associatcd witb languages with Pl VSO urder 6 (sucb as Spanish) in which the demarking of the Focus cosistructiun may end up in a vanatiun in te urdering of cunstituents as te only trace of its carlier existence as a marked Focus construction. Furtbermure, Dik wisbcs tu include the previuus demarking process in a more general principIe wbicb states ubat any pragmatically 246 Reseñas marked constructiun may undergo markedness sbift, finally leading tu a pragmaticalí>’ unmarked, neutral construction type. This rnay again be applied tu Spanish, in whicb constructions whicb were formerly pragmaticalí>’ marked (and whicb are currentí>’ so in otber languages sucb as English) bave becume grammaticalised (neutral) constructiosis in sume contexts. Left and rigbt dislocations pruvide an instance of such demarking in Spanisb ~. The following two chapters diseuss two further theuretical issues. Chapter 15 describes asid explains certain discrepancies between undcrlying clause structure asid surface expression including the cunstruction of Raising. Chapter 16 presents dxc basic notions relevant tu a tbeory of accessibility, understood as thc capacity of a term tu be ube target of sume grammatical uperation, by dealing with tbe various types of constraints explaining the isiapplicability of a particular uperation tu a particular term. Finally, a furtber set of chapters reflects the increasing interest in FO in matters pertaining tu tbe discourse and the pragmatics of verbal interaction, as can be seen by the publicatiun of two recent vulumes un FG (Cunnully et al., eds. 1996 and Bulkestein & Hanna>’, eds. furtbcuming). Wbilc chapter 18 presents a preliminary version uf a funetiosial grammar uf discourse, chapters 17 and 10 give an accuunt of two furtber issues witb implicatiusis fur tbe urganisation uf tbe discourse, extra- clausal constituents asid anapboric relatiosis, respectively. Particularly intcresting is the discussion uf extra-clausal cunstituents (ECCs) in Chapter 17. ‘[bis completes te study uf intra-clausal constituents ané pragmatic functiuns pruvided ‘si TFGI:cb.13 asid reflects ¡‘(Vs cuncern with spuken discourse. Moreuver, te discussion is most welcomc in graminatical tbeory, which bas su far devoted much less attention tu tbis typc of constitucsit than tu clausal-internal elements. Dik’s accounr uf ECCs centres un tbeir typology, in terms of positiun and, mure importantly, functiun. By and large, ECCs fuiflíl a wide range uf functions, from te interactional asid attitudinal tu tbuse of discourse organisation. Related tu the latter, Dilc distinguishes three main pragmatic functions: Boundary n-iarking, Orientatiun asid Tail. The wider pragmatic funetiun uf Orientation reformulates Dik’s functiun of ‘[heme (cf TFGI:13.l). ‘[bis isa kind of pragmatic superfunction wbicb includes otbers sucb as Themc, Condition and Setting. Dik wisbes tu make a clear distinction between Iheme (a function uutside the clause) asid Tupic (une uf the twu pragrnatic fusictiosis within te clause, te olber une heme Focus). However, if Tbeme (and, more generally, Orientation) is closely assuciated with ube notion of topicality, tben we cuuld ask what difference tbere is betwcen intra-clausal ‘[opic and extra-clausal Theme (ie. wby have two labels referring tu similar, if not the same, concept), especially when Dik himself uses tbe term “integrated Theme” fur Topic (cf. TFG2:398). ‘[he discussion is othcrwise detailed, well illustrated with examples asid illuminating. ‘[he last chapter is meant as a first step towards a grammatical model tbat takes into account levels uf linguistic organisatiun higher tban the clause. As Dik himself ts cautious enougb tu stato, tbis is indeed just tbe bare outlines of wbat a tbcory of discourse sbould look like. The discussion is centred un tbree main lines: the decisions that 5 must take ‘u building ‘sp a discourse, tIte overalí organisation of a discourse asid tbe notion of discourse coherence. ‘[be secund of these tupics considers discuurse as having a bierarchical, layered structurc. Each of the layers is Reseñas 247 represented by different types of units, wbicb are viewed as subdivisions uf a discourse event, botl¡ from tbe interactiunal puint uf view and from te puint of view uf the content. In relation tu the conccpt of coherence, te autor considers wbat factors contribute tu boub local asid global discourse coberence. Among these, Dik mesitions notions sucl¡ asframe asid script, iconicitjy, tbe use of connectors, asid the importance uf the pragmatic functions Topic asid Focus for cuberence. Despite tbe introductury character of tbe chapter. Dik dues indeed tuuch upon tbuse main features of discuurse which sbould be taken into account in a discourse grammatical model. It would be desirable tbat, in vicw of its own standards of adequacy and of te recent interests in linguistic tbeory, FO should progressively develup into such amodel. On te wbole, te buok is a successful attempt tu pruvide the reader witb botb a descriptive ant) explanatory grammar uf complex and derived constructiosis from a functional perspective. Among te drawbacks found, une could mentiun dic use of isolated, invented, and often unnatural examples, whicb seem inappropriate in a grammar whose aim is tu describe a natural language as functioning in a wider, pragmatic setting. In general, it would bave been helpful, for a greater cumprehension, tu have included more examples illustrating te theoretical discussions; tus is especially ubviuus in chapter 18. A certain degree of superficiality has been also observed in te accuunt uf sume phenumena in particular languages, as a consequence of te wish tu accuunt for as many languages as possible, in an attempt tu arrive at universal explanations. Finalí>’, sume issues which were not discussed in dept in TGFJ are left unmentiuned bere. Particularí>’ striking in tbis respect is the puor treatment given in FO tu prosudic features. Despite diese disadvantages, TFG2 is an essential text for advanced students of FO and in general fur an>’ scbolar interested in functiunal-uriented (or even ouber more formal) mudels uf language. It has been a lung-awaited aud ¡nuch-needed buuk whicb successfully completes te treatmesit uf FO given in TFGJ, by pruviding a description of a natural language tat is typological, pragmatically and psycbulugically adequate. Like ube first part, it will assuredly be te standard current reference buuk un FO for man>’ years tu come. NOTES Siewierska (1991) is an excellent critical accousit of FO and highly recommendable for any scholar in linguistics wishing to learn the working of the model. 2 Such as the Working Papers in Functional Oranunar, available frum IFOTT, Institute for Functional Research in Language and Language Use, University of Amsterdam. Ihese are Dik’s examples. Por a detailed accuunt uf the higher layers of underlying clause structure (Predication, Proposition and Clause), see TEO]: ch. 12. “ The terin operator is used in FO tu refer tu modificatiuns and modulatiuns of linguistic expressions effected by grammatical rneans (Siewierska 199 t :20). 5 This is particularly obvious in the discussions of echo-questiosis (Q-pattern strategy) and multiple Q-word questions. 6 Pl in FO is a clause-initial special pusition fur constituents witb the pragmatic function uf Topic or Focus or uther special cunstituents such as subordinators, relativepronouns, etc. 248 Reseñas The following exaniples in Spanish may serve as illustration of one of these processes (left disiocaliun ur, in Dik’s terns, Theme+Clause construction): a. La ropa ya la he lavado. b. El coche lo vendí la semana pasada. c. A mi hermano le encanta el fdtbol. REFERENCES van der Auwera, Johan and Luuis Ooosscns (eds.) (1987). Ins ant) Outs of tite Predication. Dordrecht: Foris. Bolkestein, A. Machtclt, Henk A. Cumbé, Simon C. Dik, Casper dc Groot, Jadranka Gvozdanovic and Co Vet (1981). Predicarion ant) Expression in Functional Gramman London: Academic Press. Bolkestcin, A. Macbtelt, Casper de Oruot and J. Lachían Mackenzie (eds.) (1985a). Syntax ant) Pragmatics in Functional Grammar Dordrecht: Foris. Bolkestein, A. Machtelt, Casper de Groot and J. Laeblan Mackenzie (cés.) (1985b). Predicates ant) Terms in Functional Grammar. Dordrecht: Foris. Bolkestein, Machtelt, and Mike Hannay (eds.) (forthcoming). Functional Grammar ant) Verbal Interaction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Connolí>’, Jobn FI., Roel Nl. Vismans, Cbristopher 5. Butíer asid Richard A. Gatward (eds.) (1996). Discourse ant) Pragmatics in Functional Grammar Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Dane~, Frantisek (1966). A tbree-level approach tu syntax. Travaus Linguistiques de Prague 1, 225-240. Dik, Simon C. (1978). Functional Grumman Amsterdam: North-Holland ( 3rd printing 1981, Dordrccht; Furis). Dik, Simon C. (1989). Tite TIteory of Functional Gramman Partí. TIte Síructure of the Clause. Dordrecht: Foris. (20d rey. cd., 1997, Berlin asid New York: Mouton dc Gruytcr). Duwning, Angela and Philip Locke (1992). A University Course in Englisit Gramman Heme! Hcmpstead: Prentice Hall International. Givón, Talmy (1984) and (1990). Syntax: A Functional-Typological Introduction. 2 vds. Amsterdam: John Benjarnisis. Hannay, Mike asid Elsevine Vester (eds.) (1990). Working with FG; Descriptive ant) Computational Applications. Durdrecht: Foris. Huekstra, ‘[cun, Harry van der Hulst and Michael Moortgat (eds.) (1981). Perspectives o» Functional Gramman Dordrecht: Foris. Lyons, John (1977). Semantics. 2 vols. Ca¡nbridgc: Cambridge University Prcss. Martínez Caro, Elena (1995). Funciones Pragmáticas, Orden de Constituyentes y Acentuación en Inglés y en Español. Estudio de Corpus. Tesis Doctoral. Departamento dc Filología Inglesa, Universidad Complutense de Madrid. Moreno Cabrera, Juan Carlos (1987). Fundamentos de Sintaxis General. Madrid: Síntesis. Nuyts, Jan ant) Georges dc Schutter (cds.) (1987). Getting Ones Words into Line. O» Word Order ant) Functional Grammar. L)ordrecbt: Furis. Reseñas 249 Quirk, Randolpb, Sidney Greenbaum, Oeoffrey Leecb asid Jan Svartvik (1972). A Grammar of Contemporary English. London: Longman. Sicwierska, Anna (1991). Functional Gramman London: Routledge. Elena Martínez Caro Universidad Complutense de Madrid Departamento de Filología Inglesa Facultad de Filología 28040 Madrid (SPAIN) E-mail: fling09 @ sis.ucm.es