Aviso: para depositar documentos, por favor, inicia sesión e identifícate con tu cuenta de correo institucional de la UCM con el botón MI CUENTA UCM. No emplees la opción AUTENTICACIÓN CON CONTRASEÑA
 

On the discrepant results in synchrony judgment and temporal-order judgment tasks: a quantitative model.

dc.contributor.authorGarcía Pérez, Miguel Ángel
dc.contributor.authorAlcalá Quintana, Rocío
dc.date.accessioned2023-06-20T04:05:27Z
dc.date.available2023-06-20T04:05:27Z
dc.date.issued2012-10
dc.description.abstractResearch on the perception of temporal order uses either temporal-order judgment (TOJ) tasks or synchrony judgment (SJ) tasks, in both of which two stimuli are presented with some temporal delay and observers must judge the order of presentation. Results generally differ across tasks, raising concerns about whether they measure the same processes. We present a model including sensory and decisional parameters that places these tasks in a common framework that allows studying their implications on observed performance. TOJ tasks imply specific decisional components that explain the discrepancy of results obtained with TOJ and SJ tasks. The model is also tested against published data on audiovisual temporal-order judgments, and the fit is satisfactory, although model parameters are more accurately estimated with SJ tasks. Measures of latent point of subjective simultaneity and latent sensitivity are defined that are invariant across tasks by isolating the sensory parameters governing observed performance, whereas decisional parameters vary across tasks and account for observed differences across them. Our analyses concur with other evidence advising against the use of TOJ tasks in research on perception of temporal order.
dc.description.departmentDepto. de Psicobiología y Metodología en Ciencias del Comportamiento
dc.description.facultyFac. de Psicología
dc.description.refereedTRUE
dc.description.sponsorshipMinisterio de Ciencia e Innovación (MICINN)
dc.description.statuspub
dc.eprint.idhttps://eprints.ucm.es/id/eprint/35690
dc.identifier.doi10.3758/s13423-012-0278-y
dc.identifier.issn1531-5320
dc.identifier.officialurlhttp://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0278-y
dc.identifier.relatedurlhttp://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13423-012-0278-y/fulltext.html
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/20.500.14352/44900
dc.issue.number5
dc.journal.titlePsychonomic bulletin & review
dc.language.isoeng
dc.page.final46
dc.page.initial820
dc.publisherSpringer
dc.relation.projectIDPSI2009-08800
dc.rights.accessRightsrestricted access
dc.subject.cdu159.9.07
dc.subject.keywordSynchrony judgment
dc.subject.keywordTemporal-order judgment
dc.subject.keywordPoint of Subjective simultaneity
dc.subject.keywordAudiovisual events
dc.subject.keywordExperimental methods
dc.subject.keywordStimulus type
dc.subject.ucmPsicología experimental
dc.subject.unesco6106 Psicología Experimental
dc.titleOn the discrepant results in synchrony judgment and temporal-order judgment tasks: a quantitative model.
dc.typejournal article
dc.volume.number19
dcterms.referencesAlcalá-Quintana, R., & García-Pérez, M. A. (2004). The role of parametric assumptions in adaptive Bayesian estimation. Psychological Methods, 9, 250–271. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.9.2.250 Alcalá-Quintana, R., & García-Pérez, M. A. (2011). A model for the time-order error in contrast discrimination. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1221–1248. doi:10.1080/17470218.2010.540018 Alcalá-Quintana, R., & García-Pérez, M. A. (2012). Fitting psychometric functions to simultaneity and temporal-order judgment data: matlab and R routines. Manuscript submitted for publication. Allan, L. G. (1975). The relationship between judgments of successiveness and judgments of order. Perception & Psychophysics, 18, 29–36. doi:10.3758/BF03199363 Allan, L. G., & Kristofferson, A. B. (1974). Successiveness discrimination: Two models. Perception & Psychophysics, 15, 37–46. doi:10.3758/BF03205825 Ben-Yehudah, G., Sackett, E., Malchi-Ginzberg, L., & Ahissar, M. (2001). Impaired temporal contrast sensitivity in dyslexics is specific to retain-and-compare paradigms. Brain, 124, 1381–1395. doi:10.1093/brain/124.7.1381 Colonius, H., & Diederich, A. (2011). Computing an optimal time window of audiovisual integration in focused attention tasks: Illustrated by studies on effect of age and prior knowledge. Experimental Brain Research, 212, 327–337. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2732-x Diederich, A., & Colonius, H. (2011). Modeling multisensory processes in saccadic responses: Time-window-of-integration model. In M. M. Murray & M. T. Wallace (Eds.), The neural bases of multisensory processes (pp. 253–276). Boca Raton: CRC Press. Donohue, S. E., Woldorff, M. G., & Mitroff, S. R. (2010). Video game players show more precise multisensory temporal processing abilities. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1120–1129. doi:10.3758/APP.72.4.1120 Fujisaki, W., & Nishida, S. (2009). Audio–tactile superiority over visuo–tactile and audio–visual combinations in the temporal resolution of synchrony perception. Experimental Brain Research, 198, 245–259. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-1870-x García-Pérez, M. A. (1994). Parameter estimation and goodness-of-fit testing in multinomial models. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 47, 247–282. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8317.1994.tb01037.x García-Pérez, M. A. (1998). Forced-choice staircases with fixed step sizes: Asymptotic and small-sample properties. Vision Research, 38, 1861–1881. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00340-4 García-Pérez, M. A. (2010). Denoising forced-choice detection data. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 63, 75–100. doi:10.1348/000711009X424057 García-Pérez, M. A., & Alcalá-Quintana, R. (2005). Sampling plans for fitting the psychometric function. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 8, 256–289. Retrieved from http://www.ucm.es/info/Psi/docs/journal/v8_n2_2005/art256.pdf García-Pérez, M. A., & Alcalá-Quintana, R. (2010a). The difference model with guessing explains interval bias in two-alternative forced-choice detection procedures. Journal of Sensory Studies, 25, 876–898. doi:10.1111/j.1745-459X.2010.00310.x García-Pérez, M. A., & Alcalá-Quintana, R. (2010b). Reminder and 2AFC tasks provide similar estimates of the difference limen: A reanalysis of data from Lapid, Ulrich, and Rammsayer (2008) and a discussion of Ulrich and Vorberg (2009). Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1155–1178. doi:10.3758/APP.72.4.1155 García-Pérez, M. A., & Alcalá-Quintana, R. (2011a). Improving the estimation of psychometric functions in 2AFC discrimination tasks. Frontiers in Psychology: Quantitative Psychology and Measurement, 2, 96. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00096 García-Pérez, M. A., & Alcalá-Quintana, R. (2011b). Interval bias in 2AFC detection tasks: Sorting out the artifacts. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 2332–2352. doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0167-x García-Pérez, M. A., & Alcalá-Quintana, R. (2012a). Response errors explain the failure of independent-channels models of perception of temporal order. Frontiers in Psychology: Perception Science, 3, 94. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00094 García-Pérez, M. A., & Alcalá-Quintana, R. (2012b). Shifts of the psychometric function: Distinguishing bias from perceptual effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. doi:10.1080/17470218.2012.708761 García-Pérez, M. A., Alcalá-Quintana, R., Woods, R. L., & Peli, E. (2011). Psychometric functions for detection and discrimination with and without flankers. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 829–853. doi:10.3758/s13414-010-0080-8 García-Pérez, M. A., & Núñez-Antón, V. (2001). Small-sample comparisons for power-divergence goodness-of-fit statistics for symmetric and skewed simple null hypotheses. Journal of Applied Statistics, 28, 855–874. doi:10.1080/02664760120074942 García-Pérez, M. A., & Núñez-Antón, V. (2004). Small-sample comparisons for goodness-of-fit statistics in one-way multinomials with composite hypotheses. Journal of Applied Statistics, 31, 161–181. doi:10.1080/0266476032000148849 Gilchrist, J. M., Jerwood, D., & Ismaiel, H. S. (2005). Comparing and unifying slope estimates across psychometric function models. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 1289–1303. doi:10.3758/BF03193560 Harrar, V., & Harris, L. R. (2008). The effect of exposure to asynchronous audio, visual, and tactile stimulus combinations on the perception of simultaneity. Experimental Brain Research, 186, 517–524. doi:10.1007/s00221-007-1253-0 Heath, R. A. (1984). Response time and temporal order judgement in vision. Australian Journal of Psychology, 36, 21–34. doi:10.1080/00049538408255075 Hirsh, I. J., & Sherrick, C. E., Jr. (1961). Perceived order in different sense modalities. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 423–432. doi:10.1037/h0045283 Holm, H., & Alouini, M.-S. (2004). Sum and difference of two squared correlated Nakagami variates in connection with the McKay distribution. IEEE Transactions on Communications, 52, 1367–1376. doi:10.1109/TCOMM.2004.833019 Jaśkowski, P. (1991a). Two-stage model for order discrimination. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 76–82. doi:10.3758/BF03212206 Jaśkowski, P. (1991b). Perceived onset simultaneity of stimuli with unequal durations. Perception, 20, 715–726. doi:10.1068/p200715 Jaśkowski, P. (1993). Selective attention and temporal-order judgment. Perception, 22, 681–689. doi:10.1068/p220681 Kristofferson, A. B., & Allan, L. G. (1973). Successiveness and duration discrimination. In S. Kornblum (Ed.), Attention and performance IV (pp. 737–749). New York: Academic Press. Küchler, U., & Tappe, S. (2008). On the shapes of bilateral Gamma densities. Statistics and Probability Letters, 78, 2478–2484. doi:10.1016/j.spl.2008.02.039 Landerl, K., & Willburger, E. (2010). Temporal processing, attention, and learning disorders. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 393–401. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.03.008 Lin, L. I.-K. (1989). A concordance correlation coefficient to evaluate reproducibility. Biometrics, 45, 255–268. doi:10.2307/2532051 Mitrani, L., Shekerdjiiski, S., & Yakimoff, N. (1986). Mechanisms and asymmetries in visual perception of simultaneity and temporal order. Biological Cybernetics, 54, 159–165. doi:10.1007/BF00356854 Nicholls, M. E. R., Lew, M., Loetscher, T., & Yates, M. J. (2011). The importance of response type to the relationship between temporal order and numerical magnitude. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1604–1613. doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0114-x Numerical Algorithms Group. (1999). NAG Fortran Library Manual, Mark 19. Oxford: Author. Occelli, V., Spence, C., & Zampini, M. (2011). Audiotactile interactions in temporal perception. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 429–454. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0070-4 Peli, E., & García-Pérez, M. A. (1997). Contrast sensitivity in dyslexia: Deficit or artifact? Optometry and Vision Science, 74, 986–988. doi:10.1097/00006324-199712000-00017 Ram-Tsur, R., Faust, M., & Zivotofsky, A. Z. (2006). Sequential processing deficits of reading disabled persons is independent of inter-stimulus interval. Vision Research, 46, 3949–3960. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2006.07.001 Schneider, K. A., & Bavelier, D. (2003). Components of visual prior entry. Cognitive Psychology, 47, 333–366. doi:10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00035-5 Shore, D. I., Spry, E., & Spence, C. (2002). Confusing the mind by crossing the hands. Cognitive Brain Research, 14, 153–163. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00070-8 Spence, C., Baddeley, R., Zampini, M., James, R., & Shore, D. I. (2003). Multisensory temporal order judgments: When two locations are better than one. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 318–328. doi:10.3758/BF03194803 Spence, C., & Parise, C. (2010). Prior-entry: A review. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 364–379. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.12.001 Stelmach, L. B., & Herdman, C. M. (1991). Directed attention and perception of temporal order. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 539–550. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.17.2.539 Stone, J. V., Hunkin, N. M., Porrill, J., Wood, R., Keeler, V., Beanland, M., . . . Porter, N. R. (2001). When is now? Perception of simultaneity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 268, 31–38. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1326 Sternberg, S., & Knoll, R. L. (1973). The perception of temporal order: Fundamental issues and a general model. In S. Kornblum (Ed.), Attention and performance IV (pp. 629–685). New York: Academic Press. Townsend, J. T., & Colonius, H. (2005). Variability of the MAX and MIN statistic: A theory of the quantile spread as a function of sample size. Psychometrika, 70, 759–772. doi:10.1007/s11336-001-0916-1 Ulrich, R. (1987). Threshold models of temporal-order judgments evaluated by a ternary response task. Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 224–239. doi:10.3758/BF03203074 van Eijk, R. L. J., Kohlrausch, A., Juola, J. F., & van de Par, S. (2008). Audiovisual synchrony and temporal order judgments: Effects of experimental method and stimulus type. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 955–968. doi:10.3758/PP.70.6.955 van Eijk, R. L. J., Kohlrausch, A., Juola, J. F., & van de Par, S. (2010). Temporal order judgment criteria are affected by synchrony judgment sensitivity. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 2227–2235. doi:10.3758/APP.72.8.2227 Vatakis, A., Navarra, J., Soto-Faraco, S., & Spence, C. (2008). Audiovisual temporal adaptation of speech: Temporal order versus simultaneity judgments. Experimental Brain Research, 185, 521–529. doi:10.1007/s00221-007-1168-9 Vroomen, J., & Keetels, M. (2010). Perception of intersensory synchrony: A tutorial review. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 871–874. doi:10.3758/APP.72.4.871 Yamamoto, S., & Kitazawa, S. (2001). Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 759–765. doi:10.1038/89559 Yarrow, K., Jahn, N., Durant, S., & Arnold, D. H. (2011). Shifts of criteria or neural timing? The assumptions underlying timing perception studies. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1518–1531. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2011.07.003 Yates, M. J., & Nicholls, M. E. R. (2011). Somatosensory prior entry assessed with temporal order judgments and simultaneity judgments. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1586–1603. doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0117-7 Zampini, M., Guest, S., Shore, D. I., & Spence, C. (2005). Audio–visual simultaneity judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 531–544. doi:10.3758/BF03193329 Zampini, M., Shore, D. I., & Spence, C. (2003). Audiovisual temporal order judgments. Experimental Brain Research, 152, 198–210. doi:10.1007/s00221-003-1536-z
dspace.entity.typePublication
relation.isAuthorOfPublicatione5c3695e-f861-4397-94d7-7aa543f0a630
relation.isAuthorOfPublication0a7dbcf6-8a0b-4b47-91af-79bb5db7bb52
relation.isAuthorOfPublication.latestForDiscoverye5c3695e-f861-4397-94d7-7aa543f0a630

Download

Original bundle

Now showing 1 - 1 of 1
Loading...
Thumbnail Image
Name:
On the discrepant results alcala-quintana y garcía-Perez.pdf
Size:
1.31 MB
Format:
Adobe Portable Document Format

Collections