Aviso: por motivos de mantenimiento y mejora del repositorio, mañana martes día 13 de mayo, entre las 9 y las 14 horas, Docta Complutense, no funcionará con normalidad. Disculpen las molestias.
 

The left visual field attentional advantage: No evidence of different speeds of processing across visual hemifields.

dc.contributor.authorGarcía Pérez, Miguel Ángel
dc.contributor.authorAlcalá Quintana, Rocío
dc.date.accessioned2023-06-18T06:49:58Z
dc.date.available2023-06-18T06:49:58Z
dc.date.issued2015-12
dc.description.abstractTemporal-order judgment (TOJ) and simultaneity judgment (SJ) tasks are used to study differences in speed of processing across sensory modalities, stimulus types, or experimental conditions. Matthews and Welch (2015) reported that observed performance in SJ and TOJ tasks is superior when visual stimuli are presented in the left visual field (LVF) compared to the right visual field (RVF), revealing an LVF advantage presumably reflecting attentional influences. Because observed performance reflects the interplay of perceptual and decisional processes involved in carrying out the tasks, analyses that separate out these influences are needed to determine the origin of the LVF advantage. We re-analyzed the data of Matthews and Welch (2015) using a model of performance in SJ and TOJ tasks that separates out these influences. Parameter estimates capturing the operation of perceptual processes did not differ between hemifields by these analyses, whereas parameter estimates capturing the operation of decisional processes differed. In line with other evidence, perceptual processing also did not differ between SJ and TOJ tasks. Thus, the LVF advantage occurs with identical speeds of processing in both visual hemifields. If attention is responsible for the LVF advantage, it does not exert its influence via prior entry.
dc.description.departmentDepto. de Psicobiología y Metodología en Ciencias del Comportamiento
dc.description.facultyFac. de Psicología
dc.description.refereedTRUE
dc.description.sponsorshipMinisterio de Economía y Competitividad (MINECO)
dc.description.statuspub
dc.eprint.idhttps://eprints.ucm.es/id/eprint/35685
dc.identifier.doi10.1016/j.concog.2015.08.004
dc.identifier.issn1090-2376
dc.identifier.officialurlhttp://dx.doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.concog.2015.08.004
dc.identifier.relatedurlhttp://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810015300209
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/20.500.14352/24343
dc.journal.titleConsciousness and cognition
dc.language.isoeng
dc.page.final26
dc.page.initial16
dc.publisherElsevier
dc.relation.projectIDPSI2012-32903
dc.rights.accessRightsrestricted access
dc.subject.cdu159.9.07
dc.subject.keywordSimultaneity
dc.subject.keywordTemporal order
dc.subject.keywordTiming processes
dc.subject.keywordDecisional processes
dc.subject.ucmPsicología experimental
dc.subject.unesco6106 Psicología Experimental
dc.titleThe left visual field attentional advantage: No evidence of different speeds of processing across visual hemifields.
dc.typejournal article
dc.volume.number37
dcterms.referencesAlcalá-Quintana, R., & García-Pérez, M. A. (2013). Fitting model-based psychometric functions to simultaneity and temporal-order judgment data: Matlab and R routines. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 972–998. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0325-2. Allik, J., Toom, M., & Rauk, M. (2014). Detection and identification of spatial offset: Double-judgment psychophysics revisited. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 76(8), 2575–2583. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-014-0717-0. Bradley, E. L., & Blackwood, L. G. (1989). Comparing paired data: A simultaneous test for means and variances. American Statistician, 43(4), 234–235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00031305.1989.10475665. Capa, R. L., Duval, C. Z., Blaison, D., & Giersch, A. (2014). Patients with schizophrenia selectively impaired in temporal order judgments. Schizophrenia Research, 156(1), 51–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2014.04.001. Colonius, H., & Diederich, A. (2011). Computing an optimal time window of audiovisual integration in focused attention tasks: Illustrated by studies on effect of age and prior knowledge. Experimental Brain Research, 212, 327–337. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2732-x. Diederich, A., & Colonius, H. (2015). The time window of multisensory integration: Relating reaction times and judgments of temporal order. Psychological Review, 122(2), 232–241. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038696. García-Pérez, M. A., & Alcalá-Quintana, R. (2012a). On the discrepant results in synchrony judgment and temporal-order judgment tasks: A quantitative model. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19(5), 820–846. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0278-y. García-Pérez, M. A., & Alcalá-Quintana, R. (2012b). Response errors explain the failure of independent-channels models of perception of temporal order. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 94. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00094. García-Pérez, M. A., & Alcalá-Quintana, R. (2013). Shifts of the psychometric function: Distinguishing bias from perceptual effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(2), 319–337. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.708761. García-Pérez, M. A., & Alcalá-Quintana, R. (2015). Converging evidence that common timing processes underlie temporal-order and simultaneity judgments: A model-based analysis. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 77, 1750–1766. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-015-0869-6. García-Pérez, M. A., & Peli, E. (2014). The bisection point across variants of the task. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 76(6), 1671–1697. http://dx.doi. org/10.3758/s13414-014-0672-9. Heath, R. A. (1984). Response time and temporal order judgement in vision. Australian Journal of Psychology, 36, 21–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00049538408255075. Keetels, M., & Vroomen, J. (2012). Perception of synchrony between the senses. In M. M. Murray & M. T. Wallace (Eds.), The neural bases of multisensory processes (pp. 147–177). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Kelly, J. G., & Matthews, N. (2011). Attentional oblique effect when judging simultaneity. Journal of Vision, 11(6), 1–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/11.6.10 (10). Kohlrausch, A., van Eijk, R., Juola, J. F., Brandt, I., & van de Par, S. (2013). Apparent causality affects perceived simultaneity. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 75(7), 1366–1373. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-013-0531-0. Kuling, I. A., van Eijk, R. L. J., Juola, J. F., & Kohlrausch, A. (2012). Effects of stimulus duration on audio-visual synchrony perception. Experimental Brain Research, 221(4), 403–412. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3182-9. Lalanne, L., van Assche, M., & Giersch, A. (2012). When predictive mechanisms go wrong: Disordered visual synchrony thresholds in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(3), 506–513. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbq107. Lalanne, L., van Assche, M., Wang, W., & Giersch, A. (2012). Looking forward: An impaired ability in patients with schizophrenia? Neuropsychologia, 50(12), 2736–2744. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.023. Matsuzaki, K. S., Kadota, H., Aoyama, T., Takeuchi, S., Sekiguchi, H., Kochiyama, T., et al (2014). Distinction between neural correlates of audiovisual temporal order and simultaneity judgments. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 94(2), 193. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.08.801. Matthews, N., Vawter, M., & Kelly, J. G. (2012). Right hemifield deficits in judging simultaneity: A perceptual learning study. Journal of Vision, 12 (2), 1, 1–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/12.2.1. Matthews, N., & Welch, L. (2015). Left visual field attentional advantage in judging simultaneity and temporal order. Journal of Vision, 15 (2), 7, 1–13. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1167/15.2.7. Mihaylova, M., Stomonyakov, V., & Vassilev, A. (1999). Peripheral and central delay in processing high spatial frequencies: Reaction time and VEP latency studies. Vision Research, 39, 699–705. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00165-5. Regener, P., Love, S., Petrini, K., & Pollick, F. (2014). Audiovisual processing differences in autism spectrum disorder revealed by a model based analysis of simultaneity and temporal order judgments. Journal of Vision, 14(10), 429. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/14.10.429. Robles, L., & Ruggero, M. A. (2001). Mechanisms of the mammalian cochlea. Physiological Reviews, 81, 1305–1352. Spence, C., & Parise, C. (2010). Prior-entry: A review. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(1), 364–379. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.12.001. Ulrich, R. (1987). Threshold models of temporal-order judgments evaluated by a ternary response task. Perception and Psychophysics, 42(3), 224–239. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03203074. van Eijk, R. L. J., Kohlrausch, A., Juola, J. F., & van de Par, S. (2008). Audiovisual synchrony and temporal order judgments: Effects of experimental method and stimulus type. Perception and Psychophysics, 70(6), 955–968. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/PP.70.6.955.
dspace.entity.typePublication
relation.isAuthorOfPublicatione5c3695e-f861-4397-94d7-7aa543f0a630
relation.isAuthorOfPublication0a7dbcf6-8a0b-4b47-91af-79bb5db7bb52
relation.isAuthorOfPublication.latestForDiscoverye5c3695e-f861-4397-94d7-7aa543f0a630

Download

Original bundle

Now showing 1 - 1 of 1
Loading...
Thumbnail Image
Name:
Left visual alacla-quintana y García -Pérez.pdf
Size:
1.01 MB
Format:
Adobe Portable Document Format

Collections